Tuesday, 18 October 2016

Zach transcript comparison essay



We would expect fluency of speech to improve and develop as the child grows older, although in the second transcript there are instances when Zach's language seems less fluent than the previous year. We could suggest various reasons as to why this is the case in relation to contextual factors such as the fact that in this transcript Zach is playing and being imaginative and therefore a lot of what he is saying is being made up on the spot. We can see an example of this where he says 'an I'm sitting here (.) to (.) for waiting (.) to get (0.5) better (.) for it (0.5)’ where examples of pauses and a false start can be seen. Alternatively it could be due to the fact we know that Zach is ‘home ill’ and it is likely this will affect his language. Despite this, the mean length of utterance (MLU) is noticeably greater in the second transcript, compared to more simple utterances in the first transcript, as we would expect, With an MLU of  0.7 in the first transcript and 1.2 in the second. In addition to suggesting Zach’s language has advanced we can draw attention to the fact that there are slightly more open questions in the second transcript and as these are likely to produce longer, more complex replies and therefore a longer MLU. However, in order to make conclusions about Zach’s language development we would need far more utterances to allow the MLU data to be both comparable and reliable.

 

We can see Zach’s development from telegraphic ‘look (.)/blown away’ to post telegraphic ‘please can you pick it up?’ showing improved ability to sustain a conversation. His language is very developed at both ages although there are occasions where he uses what Naom Chomsky calls ‘overgeneralisation’. For example Zach says ‘bolognay’ rather than ‘bolognaise’ which could be an example of him over applying the rule for making nouns plural, which in this case is an example of non-standard English. Chomsky also believes that children are born with an innate ability to learn any language, although this theory is widely criticised  for ignoring the importance of interaction, for example, Zach would not have known the correct term ‘bolognese’ without communication between him and his mother.

 

Looking at the transcripts we can see that Zach shows general understanding of the structure of a conversation. Despite occasionally needing prompts to expand on his simple utterances he does not interrupt speaker H. He shows comprehension of turn taking and adjacency pairs, for instance when speaker H uses the interrogative ‘what have you eaten today?’ and Zach replies ‘bananas’.  

 

In the second transcript Zach seems as if he is the dominant speaker, supporting most theories concerning language and gender, such as Zimmerman and West’s dominance theory. He is seen to set the agenda in both conversations and even uses back-channelling ‘We don’t need breakfast (.) it’s gonna take a little time now…’ to divert the conversation back to robots. Alternatively it could be argued that this specific example actually contradicts Zach’s dominance within this conversation with his use of a cloaked imperative. Tannen says that is in fact women who use cloaked imperatives and this suggests deficiency in speech. Although with lack of context this is clearly a command and is likely to influence speaker H’s behavior, I would say it is an example of representational speech rather than regulatory speech purely because of the use of a cloaked imperative.

 

It is evident from both transcripts that there are various efforts made by speaker H to aid the child’s language development. On various occasions speaker H, the mother, models the standard form of the word after the child has used the incorrect form. For example, again Z says ‘bolognay’ and the mother repeats back ‘bolognaise’ and the pronunciation of  ‘through’ as ‘frough’ is corrected in order to help the child’s learning. We can relate this to Deb Roy’s well renowned theory that parents adapt their language to be more simplistic around children, allowing them to understand and learn. Speaker H uses very straightforward utterances with few words, and is often seen echoing words that Z has previously used to reinforce them.  Numerous prompts are used throughout both essays to encourage the child to talk and elaborate on what he is saying for example ‘’Z: What are you drawing (.) Zach?” The use of the direct address ‘Zach’ also identifies that the utterance is directed at him and therefore making him more likely to answer.

Monday, 17 October 2016

Methodology plan

Methodology


Theory


  • Deborah Cameron, spoke about how our language is influenced by and influences gender stereotypes in her book ‘The Myth of Mars and Venus’. She challenges "the idea that sex-differences might have biological rather than social causes.
  • How the language of the media creates these stereotypes form a very young but these differences are not pre-determined.
  • Nature vs nurture debate


Reliability


  • 3 girls magazines from the different companies 
  • All recent (Oct/Nov) temporal validity
  • Recent, English – to avoid differences in culture and cultural changes across time e.g. ideas about gender have considerably changed in recent years – still some issues.
  • Reliable gender theories – well accounted/well known


Comparability


  • All recent (Oct/Nov)
  • Directed at a similar audience (Age)
  • Same price (£3.99)
  • Similar availability


Ethicality


  • Published – no issues with privacy

Thursday, 6 October 2016

Possible coursework ideas

  1. The language of the media
  • attitudes-how do people feel about this language?
  • power of media text
  • criticism of language use
  • hidden meanings?
  • portrayal of different social groups, gender, races, religions? etc.
  • semantics
  • stereotypes
  • Encoding and decoding
  • denotation and connotation
Gender and the media theory

Monday, 3 October 2016

Zach drawing a banana transcript analysis

  • Virtuous mistake 'I cutting round the edge'
  • Non fluency features before more challenging words '(.) banana'
  • Requesting action 'where's my little pad gone?
  • 'what can the banana have' - imaginative
  • 'don't want to eat skins' showing contextual knowledge
  • fillers 'erm'
  • correcting/modelling adult form Z: frough H: through
  • Repetion and labelling Z: Daddy's dinner
  • Heuristic - learning of environment 'what's this lasagne'

Accent and Dialect

http://www.universalteacher.org.uk/lang/britishisles.htm
http://www.putlearningfirst.com/language/research/cheshire.html


Dialect describes a language variety where a user's regional or social background appears in his or her use of vocabulary and grammar


Accent denotes the features of pronunciation (the speech sounds) that show regional or social identity




  1. Jenny Cheshire, Reading
Jenny Cheshire used long-term participant observation to gain data about the relationship between use of grammatical variables and adherence to peer group culture by boys and girls in Reading. She gained acceptance from three groups(two of boys, one of girls) in two adventure playgrounds in Reading and recorded how often they used each of the 11 variables.


For the girls she made a distinction between the girls who did not have positive attitudes to such group activities and carrying weapons, fighting , participation in minor criminal activities, preferred job, dress and hairstyle and use of swearing.


1. non-standard -sThey calls me all the names under the sun
 2. non-standard hasYou just has to do what the teachers tell you.
 3. non-standard wasYou was with me, wasn't you?
 4. negative concordIt ain't got no pedigree or nothing.
 5. non-standard neverI never went to school today.
 6. non-standard whatAre you the little bastards what hit my son over the head?
 7. non-standard doShe cadges, she do.
 8. non-standard comeI come down here yesterday.
 9. ain't = auxiliary haveI ain't seen my Nan for nearly seven years.
 10. ain't = auxiliary beCourse I ain't going to the Avenue.
 11. ain't = copulaYou ain't no boss.






  Group A girlsGroup B girls
 non-s 5 25.84 57.27
 non-s has 36.36 35.85
 non-s was 63.64 80.95
 negative concord 12.5 58.7
 non-s never 45.45 41.07
 non-s what 33.33 5.56
 non-s come 30.77 90.63
 ain't = copula 14.29 67.12




Similar findings when boys against boys and when boys against girls were compared, showed clearly that those who conformed to the conventions of the group also used the linguistic standards of the group - and that conforming boys conformed most of all.




    


    2.  Labov, Martha's Vineyard




Martha's vineyard is an island on the East Coast of the United States of America, with a permanent population of about 6000. However, much to the disgust of a number of locals, over 40,000 visitors, know as the 'summer people' flood in every summer. The Eastern part on the island is more densely populated by the permanent residents, and is the area mostly visitor visited by the summer visitors. This is known as Down Island.  The Western third - where most of the original inhabitants live is knows as the Up-Island.
A small group of fishermen began to exaggerate a tendency already existing in their speech. They did this seemingly subconsciously, in order to establish themselves as an independent social group with superior status to the despised summer visitors. The innovation gradually became the norm for those living on the island.




    
     3.  Labov, Fourth Floor




Labov's research in the Lower East Side on New York City showed that individual speech patterns were part of a highly systematic structure of social and stylistic stratification. He studied how often the final or preconsonantal (r) was sounded in words like guard, bare and beer. use of this variable has considerable prestige in New York City. It can be measured very precisely, and its high frequency in speech makes it possible to collect data quickly.


One self-contained part of the research has become particularly well known. The speech of sales assistants in three Manhattan stores, drawn from the top(Saks), middle (Macy's) and bottom (Klein's) of the price and fashion scale. Each unwitting informant was approached with a factual enquiry designed to elicit the answer 'Fourth floor" - which may or may not contain the variable final or preconsonantal (r). The findings were that the sales assistants from Saks used it most, those from Klein's used it least and those from Macy's showed the greatest upward shift when they were asked to repeat. Frequency of use of the prestige variable final or preconsonantal (r) varied with level of formality and social class.